In the last four decades much has changed in the game of golf, but at least one constant has
been Augusta National. Armed with data on (almost) every playing of The Masters since 1983,
we try to shed some light on what it takes to win around the storied Georgia layout.
For this Masters preview, we do three distinct analyses with the aim of deepening our understanding of so-called
"course fit" — that is, the degree to which a given golf course favours a specific skill set — at
Augusta National. The first section
looks at the skill profile of past winners at Augusta National and how it compares to the canonical PGA Tour
winner; the second section analyzes
hole-level fit at The Masters, and how this has evolved
since 1983; the third, and final, section
provides an interesting addition to
past work
we've done on distance
and performance in professional golf by analyzing how this relationship has
evolved at Augusta National over time.
The winning measurements
Our
course fit tool uses the
full field
to determine how specific skills, such as driving distance or driving accuracy, are differentially rewarded
at specific courses. Here, we narrow our focus to past
winners at Augusta National; while this may be less
useful from a prediction standpoint, we can probably all agree it is at least as interesting.
In the visualization below, we have estimated golfers' skill in each of five attributes — driving distance,
driving accuracy, strokes-gained approach, strokes-gained around-the-green, and strokes-gained putting —
before
each round played since 2004 on the PGA Tour.
Each attribute has been normalized to have a mean of 0 and a standard deviation of 1
in each season. For example, at the 2012 Masters, Bubba Watson's value for driving distance
was equal to 3.4, indicating
that we estimated Watson was 3.4 standard deviations longer than the average PGA Tour player
heading into the
2012 Masters. (Recall
the
68-95-99.7 rule; if you are
3 SDs above the average in some attribute, that means you are roughly better than 99.85% of the population.) The attribute
profile for each Masters winner since 2004 is shown on the plot, in addition to the average profile of all PGA Tour winners
since 2004; spend some time clicking around.
Masters Champion Characteristics
Plot Info
SHOW AVG MASTERS WINNER
Avg. PGA TOUR Player
Avg. PGA TOUR Winner
Avg. Masters Champ
2019 Tiger Woods
There are many interesting things to note here. First, looking at the skill profile of the average PGA
Tour winner — indicated by the solid black polygon — we see that their driving accuracy is only
slightly above Tour average. Driving accuracy is a declining but still important skill on the PGA Tour,
as the final section of this
blog will show,
but winners over the last 15 years have not been particularly accurate.
You might be able to think of a few superstars that are driving
this result. Compared to the typical PGA Tour winner, Masters champions (use the button
above the graph to display their average profile) have tended to be better in all
attributes except driving accuracy, where they are actually below average. (A minor technical note: all attributes
are also made mean-zero at each event, making their interpretation as SDs better than the average player
in that field.) As you would expect, the clear misfit among recent Masters winners is Zach Johnson, while 2006
Phil Mickelson is as close as we've come to a prototypical Masters champion since 2004. It's also enlightening
to be reminded of the past skill profiles of certain players: even by Tiger's standards,
the
2005 version
of himself was pretty ridiculous,
while
2012-2014
Bubba Watson's driving was at a level even Bryson has not yet reached.
To wrap up this section, here are the 5 golfers in this year's field that most closely
match the average skill profile of past Masters champions (in order of similarity):
Patrick Cantlay, Dustin Johnson, Xander Schauffele, Adam Scott, and Justin Thomas. Interestingly,
both our clear Masters favourite in Jon Rahm, as well as the betting market's favourite in Bryson DeChambeau,
are missing from this list.
Digging deeper: hole-level fit
Now that we have some idea of which types of players tend to succeed at The Masters,
let's break it down further. The table below shows the expected increase in strokes-gained
from improving one standard deviation (SD) in driving distance, and (separately) from improving one SD
in driving accuracy, for each hole at Augusta National. The data used spans the 1983-2019
editions of The Masters. As detailed above, we have estimates for golfers' skill in each attribute
at each point in time; the values in the table are then obtained by
correlating strokes-gained at the hole level with a golfer's
attributes. Importantly, these estimates are
conditional on the golfer's
other attributes.
To put it simply, we try to answer the question: if Phil Mickelson improves his driving distance by 1 standard
deviation — roughly 8.3 yards on the 2020 PGA Tour —
while the rest of his skill set remains as is (difficult to
imagine, we know!),
how much would we expect his strokes-gained to improve on each hole?
For reference, at a randomly selected hole on the PGA Tour from 1983-2019, the benefit of a 1
SD increase in distance was roughly 0.025 strokes, while for accuracy it was 0.02 strokes.
To view how the hole-specific estimates have changed over time, click a row and an evolution
plot will expand. The final two columns list the golfers in this year's Masters field
who stand to benefit the
most, and least, from each hole's respective "fit" — if you've followed Weir's
shot tracker over the past few years, it shouldn't come as a surprise that Hole 2
doesn't exactly suit his game.
Which holes, which skills?
SG INCREASE FROM 1 SD IMPROVEMENT IN:
WHO BENEFITS?
There is a lot to digest from this table, here are our main takeaways:
1) As expected, the par 5s at Augusta National provide the greatest advantage to golfers with
length, and Hole 15 tops the list.
2) Hole 9 is the par 4 with the largest reward for distance; in fact, if we restrict our attention
to the last 10 years, the 9th hole provides the largest distance advantage of any hole.
This was surprising,
but makes sense given the trees on either side of the fairway end
abruptly at the bottom of the hill, making it critical to either drive it past them,
or be in the fairway further back.
3) Hole 3 — despite being drivable — does not seem to favour
players with more distance (or accuracy). The low value for both attributes
appears to be due to the fact that, historically,
Hole 3 has had
low variance
in scores.
One possible explanation for this is that the green and surrounding runoffs are so treacherous
that players are frequently forced to play away from the hole, increasing the likelihood of pars.
4) While driving accuracy is in general rewarded less at Augusta National than at other
PGA Tour venues, it does earn above-average returns on
Holes 2, 10, and
7.
5) Since 1983, Holes 9, 13, and 18 have seen the largest increases in the advantage they give to longer golfers.
Finally, a few points of clarification. We restrict our attention to the effects of driving
distance and accuracy over the full time period because the strokes-gained categories are only available
after 2004;
if both driving accuracy and driving distance have low value on a given hole,
this just means that skill (to the degree that driving distance and accuracy measure it)
is not rewarded on that hole; a negative estimate in an individual year can arise
simply due to statistical noise: sometimes lower-skilled golfers beat higher-skilled golfers!
Distance or accuracy? Taking the long view
In this final section, we take a step back from our hole-level analysis to understand
how the overall relationship between distance, accuracy, and performance has evolved
at Augusta National over the last four decades. Before focusing on Augusta however, let's look at the
relationship using data from all
PGA Tour events.
As with the hole-level analysis, the focus here is on
the expected increase in total strokes-gained from improving driving distance, or driving accuracy, by
1 standard deviation. Further, as above, these estimates attempt — through regression analysis —
to hold constant,
or control for, the effects of the other attributes. For example, the data point for driving distance in 2015
in the plot below tells us the following: if a golfer increases their driving distance
by 1 standard deviation (which equalled
~8.4 yards in 2015), then they can expect their total strokes-gained to increase by ~0.45 strokes per round.
The key here is that driving accuracy, strokes-gained approach, etc, are
held constant.
Similarly, the accuracy data point for 2015 tells us the expected increase in strokes-gained from improving
driving accuracy by 1 SD (~4.4% in 2015), while holding constant driving distance, etc. For the years
before 2004, where the strokes-gained categories were not available, we can only control
for driving accuracy when estimating the distance effect (and vice versa), but by assuming
the correlation structure between the five attributes remained constant over time
(a tenuous assumption), we can make the pre-2004 and post-2004 estimates comparable.
Let's pull ourselves out of the statistical weeds: what should you take away from this? We see that
before 1987, the benefit to a golfer from improving in distance
or accuracy were roughly similar; conversely, while the positive effect of added distance has remained steady since then,
the same cannot be said for accuracy. In recent years, improving by 1 SD in distance
is estimated to be worth about 0.1-0.15 strokes more than the same improvement in the
accuracy department.
This complements our
previous analysis on
this subject — which I encourage interested readers to visit — where we showed
that the raw correlation between driving accuracy and performance has declined markedly since 1983, while the same
correlation for distance has increased. The results shown here provide an explanation:
longer players today still enjoy the same benefit from added distance as bombers of the past,
but the penalty for
inaccuracy has declined (this is just the flip side to the benefit of increased accuracy declining). Because
longer players tend to be more inaccurate, this explains the rising raw correlation between
distance and performance. An analogous explanation goes through for driving accuracy. (For more thoughts
on this analysis, see the Endnote.)
As promised, the next plot displays results from an identical analysis to the one described above, except only
using data from Augusta National. Due to the smaller sample size, there is much more variation in the
yearly estimates; despite the noise, some clear patterns emerge. Averaging over the entirety of the sample, the
advantage of distance is larger at Augusta National than at the average PGA Tour event; this has been
increasing over time, with the recent distance advantage hovering around 0.6 strokes (compared to the
PGA Tour average of ~0.45). For driving accuracy,
there is no clear trend, with accuracy being rewarded less at Augusta National than other PGA Tour venues
for the duration of the time period.
The data displayed here is just the aggregated view of the earlier hole-level analysis. That is, in each
year the sum of the hole-level estimates will equal the round-level estimate. As mentioned earlier,
it seems the rising importance of distance at Augusta National has been driven mainly by Holes 9, 13, and
18.